Egyptian and Semitic in Uto-Aztecan
Click here for the full PDF article Egyptian and Semitic in Uto-Aztecan
Excerpts from the 400-page book Exploring the Explanatory Power of Egyptian and Semitic in Uto-Aztecan
by Brian D. Stubbs, January 2015
Many unresolved questions in Uto-Aztecan (UA) have eluded linguists for the century since Sapir (1913, 1915) established UA as a language family, and while the language ties in this title may seem unseemly to some, they explain more of UA’s previous unknowns than many might be comfortable with initially. So take your time.
Uto-Aztecan consists of some 30 related languages in the western U.S. and western Mexico, from the Utes in the north to the Aztecs in the south. More than 1500 correlations between UA and Egyptian, Hebrew, and Aramaic, consistent with the linguistic comparative method, create a case stronger than the first accepted treatise establishing each Native American language family. Yet like all new knowledge that answers many questions, it also raises new questions.
Knowing how unwelcome such a proposal would be in the linguistic community and being a peace-loving recluse by nature, I was in no hurry to invite the avalanche of controversy upon me. However, equally risky is pressing my luck in postponing a presentation that should preferably occur on this side of the mortal divide. So as youth becomes a more distant memory, it is time to share these findings, which, as both a Semiticist and a Uto-Aztecanist, I could not help but notice. Such observations surfaced during a three-decade effort to write the reference book Uto-Aztecan: A Comparative Vocabulary (UACV, Stubbs 2011), favorably received among Uto-Aztecanists, though no two Uto-Aztecan specialists will agree on all aspects and reconstructions, as Kenneth Hill notes in a favorable review in the International Journal of American Linguistics (Hill 2012), and after any linguistic comparative work, adjustments inevitably follow. A case not valid will unravel with scrutiny, while truth is further substantiated with time, accumulating more and more supports.
The book is intended for linguists, Semiticists, and Egyptologists, and therefore includes the linguistic rigor demanded by the comparative method, and for non-specialists it also contains introductions to linguistics (language science), to the Semitic languages, and to UA. This article is a mere handful of highlights—260 examples from 1528—a quick glance in a nutshell.
After Sapir (1913, 1915) established Uto-Aztecan as a viable family of related languages, Voegelin, Voegelin, and Hale (1962) produced the first numbered list of 171 cognate sets (groups of related words). Klar (1977) brought the Chumash languages to clarity with 168 sets. Taylor (1963) established Caddoan (a language family of the central plains), assembling 107 cognate sets. Hale (1962, 1967) did the definitive study for Kiowa-Tanoan with 99 sets. This work’s proposal may better compare to tying two distant language families, as did Haas (1958) by ending four decades of controversy in uniting Algonkian-Ritwan, an eastern U.S. family with a west coast family, by means of 93 sets. Chamberlain (1888) began the union of Catawba with Siouan via 17 comparisons, and Siebert (1945) secured it with mostly morphological correlations, as not enough clear cognate sets were known at the time to establish correspondences (Campbell 1997, 140). Thus, the going rate is between 50 and 200 sets to establish most Native American language families. So this case of 1500-plus sets merits proportionate consideration.
The Cognate Collections in Chronological Order and Their Abbreviations
(Branch cognate collections are abbreviated as the initial(s) of author surname(s) dot branch; only the six in bold address the whole language family)
Sapir Sapir’s “Southern Paiute and Nahuatl: a Study in Uto-Aztecan” (1913, 1915)
VVH Voegelin, Voegelin, and Hale’s Typological and Comparative Grammar of UA (1962)
B.Tep Burton Bascom’s Proto-Tepiman (1965)
M67 Wick Miller’s Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets (1967)
BH.Cup William Bright and Jane Hill’s “The Linguistic History of the Cupeño” IJAL 33 (1967)
HH.Cup Jane Hill and Kenneth Hill’s “Stress in the Cupan Languages” IJAL 34 (1968)
I.Num David Iannucci’s Numic Historical Phonology (1972)
CL.Azt Campbell and Langacker’s “Proto-Aztecan Vowels,” IJAL 44 (1978)
Fowler83 Catherine Fowler’s “Lexical Clues to UA Prehistory” IJAL 49 (1983) and her fieldnotes
L.Son Andrés Lionnet’s Relaciones Internas de la Rama Sonorense (1985)
M88 Wick Miller’s 1988 Computerized Database of Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets (1988)
Munro.Cup Pamelo Munro’s “Stress and Vowel Length in Cupan Absolute Nouns” IJAL 56 (1990)
KH.NUA Kenneth Hill’s Serrano Dictionary, with comparative notes relevant to NUA (2001)
KH/M06 Kenneth Hill’s Miller’s Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets: revised and expanded by KCH (2006)
UACV Brian Stubbs’ Uto-Aztecan: A Comparative Vocabulary (2011)
The Uto-Aztecan Languages, Abbreviations, Branches, and Branch Abbreviations
Mn Mono (Western Numic) NP Northern Paiute (WNum)
TSh Tümpisha Shoshoni (CNum) Sh Shoshoni (Central Numic) WSh Western Shoshoni (CNum) Cm Comanche (CNum)
Kw Kawaiisu (Southern Numic)
Hp Hopi (single language branch) Eu Tb Tübatülabal (single language branch) Op Ls Luiseño (Takic) Tbr Ca Cahuilla (Takic) Yq Cp Cupeño (Takic) AYq Sr Serrano (Takic) My Gb Gabrielino (Takic) Wr Ktn Kitanemuk (Takic) Tr TO Tohono O’odham (Tepiman) WTr UP Upper Pima/Pima Alto (Tep) Cr Nv Nevome (Tepiman) Wc LP Lower Pima/Pima Bajo (Tep) CN
Eudeve (Opatan)
Opata (Opatan)
Tubar (single language branch) Yaqui (Cahitan)
Arizona Yaqui (Cah)
Mayo (Cahitan)
Guarijio (Tarahumaran) Tarahumara (Tarahumaran) Western Tr (Tarahumaran) Cora (Corachol) Huichol (Corachol)
Ch Chemehuevi
SP Southern Paiute
WMU White Mesa Ute
NU Northern/Uintah Ute (SNum)
CU Colorado Ute
(SNum)
PYp Pima de Yepáchic PYc Pima de Yécora NT Northern Tepehuan ST Southern Tepehuan
(Tep) Pl
(Tep) HN (Tep)
(Tep)
(SNum) (SNum) (SNum)
Classical Nahuatl Pipil
Huastec Nahuatl
(Aztecan) (Aztecan) (Aztecan)Proto-Uto-Aztecan (PUA); i.e., Hebrew / Phoenician b > PUA *p (> means ‘became’ or ‘changed to’; < means ‘changed from’; * marks a proto-form or original sound or word as reconstructed by linguists. So Hebrew b > PUA *p means Hebrew b changed to what linguists see as originally *p in UA). The matches presented are a few from among many more examples of each sound change, though abbreviated from the fuller data and explanations in the numbered paragraph sets in the book. Remember that in language change, sounds are regularly lost: Latin fabulare > *fablar > Portuguese falar and Spanish hablar; and we no longer pronounce the initial k- nor final -e in knife. One will also notice that clusters of two consonants often reduce to one consonant, the first usually being lost: in ‘debt’ the cluster -bt- lost the first consonant -b-, no longer pronounced; -b- was also lost in Portuguese falar < *fablar; and the -l- in ‘walk’ and ‘talk’ is not pronounced. The first consonant is often absorbed to double the second: incomplete, but in-legal > illegal, and in-regular > irregular,
Some characteristics of UA are different or not at all like Egyptian or Semitic, but reflect influences rather typical of Amerindian language families, which we would expect of a transplant from the outside into the Americas. One example is suppletion in singular vs. plural verb forms. That is, one verb is used for singular subjects and an entirely different word is used when the subject is plural, yet suppletion is nearly non-existent in Semitic or Egyptian. A score of such pairs in UA show such influences on UA. Semitic conjugation morphology (patterns of how verbs are conjugated) is not productive in UA, but hundreds of fossilized forms of both the suffixed / perfective conjugation (singular yašiba; plural yašib-uu) and the prefixed / imperfective conjugation (yi-/ya-, ti-/ta-, etc) are found in UA.
In contrast to differences, other grammatical features align and substantial amounts of Uto-Aztecan vocabulary produce consistent sets of sound correspondences between UA and the Near-Eastern languages, each treated as a separate entity. Linguists know that as a language changes, each sound remains or changes consistently to one other sound in the same language and environment, and this creates a set of sound correspondences between related languages. For example, among the consistent patterns of sound correspondences, some 40 examples show Hebrew b corresponding to p of
and in-moral > immoral. The first consonant being absorbed to double the second can be seen at the top of page 4 in 871, 872, 99, 889, and at the top of page 8 in 384, 398, 434, and at the bottom of page 11 in 560 and 561.
Semitic verbs consist of three consonants (bṣq, for example) subject to a variety of vowel patterns for various verb conjugations, adjectives, and nouns. The numbers are those from the book. (C = any consonant, an unknown consonant):
Semitic b
(527) baraq ‘lightning’
(528) byt / bayit / beet ‘spend the night, house’
(528) byt / bayit / beet ‘spend the night, house’
(528) bytu ‘spend the night, plural’
(531) Hebrew boo’ ‘coming (used as ‘way to’)
(534) Hebrew batt ‘daughter’
(550) Aramaic bǝsár ‘flesh, penis’
(559) Semitic *bakay; Syriac baka’ ‘cry’
(532) Arabic bṣr ‘see’; baaṣirat ‘eye’; Hebrew *booṣer(et) > UA *pusi ‘eye’
(616) Aramaic dakar ‘male’
(617) Aramaic diqn-aa ‘beard / chin-the’ (618) Aramaic di’b-aa ‘wolf-the’
(620) unattested f. pl: *đabboot(eey) ‘flies’
> UA *taka ‘man, person’
> UA *tï’na ‘mouth’
> UA *tï’pa ‘wolf’ (< Aramaic, but not < Hebrew hazzǝ’eb) > UA *tïpputi ‘flea’
(577) ’aas- ‘myrtle willow’
(579) pa’r- ‘mouse’
(581) Hebrew ’arṣ-aa ‘earth-ward, down’
> UA *wasV ‘willow’
> UA *pu’wi(N) ‘mouse’ > UA *wïcï ‘fall’ (c = ts)
> Uto-Aztecan *p
> UA *pïrok; My berok ‘lightning’
> UA *pïtï; Tr bete ‘house’
> UA *pïtï ‘lie down, spend night’; Num *payïC ‘go home’ > UA *pïtu ‘lie down, spend the night, plural’
> UA *pooC ‘road, way, path’
> UA *pattï ‘daughter’
> UA *pisa ‘penis’
> UA *paka’ ‘cry’
(535) Aramaic bǝquuraa ‘livestock’ (540) Hebrew bṭђ / *baṭiiђ ‘trust(ed)’ (552) bṭn ‘be pregnant’
(553) bṣq ‘swell’
> UA *pukuN ‘domestic animals’ > UA *piciwa ‘believe’ (ṭ > c (=ts)) > UA *puca ‘pregnant’ (ṭ > c (=ts)) > UA *posa ‘swell’
(556) bayṣa(t) / beeṣa(t), pl: beeṣoot ‘egg, testicle’ > UA *pïyso ‘testicle’
(558) bwṣ / byḍ ‘be white’; buuṣ ‘white linen’ > UA *pos ‘white’: Tb poosït~’opoos ‘be white’ (562) -bbiiṭ ‘look’ > UA *pici / *pica ‘look, see’ (ṭ > c (=ts))
The other voiced stops also devoice, that is, Semitic b, d, g > UA p, t, k; also Semitic q > k:
(606) dubur ‘buttocks, rear’
(607) dobɛr ‘pasture, vegetation’
(1484) dwr / duur ‘go round, turn, revolve’
(1103) dakka ‘make flat, stamp, crush’
(1279) Aramaic *yagar ‘hill, heap of stones’
(608) gdʕ ‘cut off’
(57) *siggoob ‘squirrel’
(1014) qədaal ‘neck, nape of neck’
(1023) tqn ‘make straight, set, lay down’
(1089) Hebrew qippod ‘hedgehog’; Arabic *qunpuđ ‘hedgehog’ > UA *kïNpa ‘prairie dog’ (864) *quuppoot ‘baskets, pl’ > UA *koppo ‘basket’
(74) Hebrew təbuu’at ‘produce from the land’ > UA *tïpï’at / *tïpat (AMR) ‘pinion nut’
Proto-Semitic *đ (> Arabic đ, Aramaic d, Hebrew z), corresponds to UA *t:
> UA *tupur ‘hip, buttocks’
> UA *tupi ‘grass, vegetation’
> UA *tur ‘whirl, roll, twist’
> UA *takka ‘flat’
> UA *yakaC / *yakaR (AMR) ‘nose, point, ridge’
> UA *katu’ 'cut, wound'
> UA *sikkuC ‘squirrel’
> UA *kutaC ‘neck’ (*q > k)
> UA *tïkaC ‘put lying down, stretched/spread flat’ (*q > k)
Semitic ’aleph or glottal stop ’ > w in UA (which change also occurs in Arabic), or other times both a glottal stop and adjacent round vowels occur, perhaps ’ causing vowels to round (o, u):
> UA *wari ‘mountain lion’
> UA *yawamin-(o) ‘believe (him/it)’
> UA *wokoC ‘pine tree’ (C = unknown consonant) > Ls yawáywa, Sr yï’aayï’a’n ‘be pretty, beautiful’ > UA *wïsi ‘person’
(566) ’ariy / ’arii ‘lion’
(567) Hebrew ya’amiin-o ‘he believes him/it’
(569) Hebrew ’egooz ‘nut tree’
(571) Semitic ya’ya’ / yaa’ayaa’ ‘(be) beautiful’
(572) Hebrew ’iiš ‘man, person’
(574) Hebrew ’išaa / ’ešɛt / ’išt- ‘woman, wife of’ > UA *wïCti ‘woman, wife’ (C = unknown consonant)
(*q > k) (*q > k)
(575) kama’- ‘truffle(s)’ > UA *kamo’- ‘sweet potato’ (truffles are also edible fleshy appendages to a root system, as are potatoes)
(596) ’arnab ‘hare’
(576) ’atay, *’atii-; Syriac ’ita / ’ɛta ‘come’ (871) ’pl / *tu’pal ‘be dark, go down (sun), f’ (872) ’pl / *yu’pal ‘be dark, go down, m’ (873) ’pl / *yu’pal ‘be dark, go down, m’
(1110) Aramaic ’ard-aa’ ‘mushroom-the’ (1331) ’ikkaar ‘plowman, tiller of ground’ (1333) Hebrew m’n / *me’’an ‘refuse’
Semitic initial r- > t- in UA:
(600) r’y / raa’aa ‘see, v’
(603) Aramaic rima / rimǝ-taa ‘large stone-the’
(604) Aramaic rə’emaan-aa / reemaan-aa ‘antelope-the’ > UA *tïmïna 'antelope’ (99) rakb-u ‘they mounted, climbed’ > UA *tï’pu / *tïppu ‘climb up’ (889) Aramaic rakbaa / rikbaa ‘upper millstone’ > UA *tïppa ‘mortar (and/or) pestle’
Loss of Semitic final -r, without effect on the preceding vowel:
(565) makar ‘sell’
(616) dakar ‘male’
(550) Aramaic bǝsár ‘flesh, penis’
(1331) ’ikkaar ‘plowman, tiller of ground’
> UA *maka ‘give, sell’
> UA *taka ‘man, person’ > UA *pisa ‘penis’
> UA *wika 'digging stick'
> UA *wa’na ‘rabbit net’
> UA *wic ‘come’ (t > c(ts) by high vowels like i, u) > UA *tu’pa > *cuppa ‘be dark, (fire) go out’ (t > c, by u) > UA *yu’pa > *yuppa ‘be dark, black, (fire) go out’
> UA *yu’pa(l) > Aztecan *yowal, CN yowal-li ‘night, n’
Aztecan branch regularly loses a single -p- > UA *witto’oC ‘mushroom’
> UA *wika 'digging stick'
> Hp meewan- ‘forbid, warn’
> UA *tïwa ‘find, see’ > UA *tïmï-ta ‘rock’
Semitic initial voiceless pharyngeal ђ > UA *hu, or w/o/u, and non-initially ђ > w/o/u:
(672) ђbq ‘pass air, break wind’ > UA *hupak- ‘stink’ (*q > k) (673) ђnk ‘train, dedicate’; ђanukkaa ‘dedication, consecration’ > Ca huneke ‘to take an Indian bath’;
Yq húnak-te ‘show, direct, raise (young)’ (671) ђmm ‘heat, bathe, wash’ > UA *huma ‘wash, bathe’
(1040) ђml ‘carry, lift, pick up’ > UA *homa ‘take, carry, pick up’
The Semitic voiced pharyngeal ʕ > UA w/o/u, i.e., some form of rounding, as the Phoenician ʕ symbol > Greek o:
> UA *wakol 'round(ed)’
> UA *pakuwa ‘mushroom, fungus’ (*q > k)
> UA *(w)umaC / *(w)ïmaC ‘rain, be cloudy / overcast’ > UA *wowa ‘vulva, vagina’
> UA *woki ‘track, footprint’ (*q > k)
> UA *sipwa ‘finger’
(677) ʕagol ‘round’
(676) paqʕ- ‘whiteness, species of fungus’
(683) ʕmṭ ‘cloud over, become dark’
(686) ʕɛrwaa ‘nakedness, genitals’
(1197) Hebrew ʕaaqeeb ‘heel, footprint’
(747) Aramaic / Syriac ṣibʕ- ‘finger’
(876) dʕk, impfv: -dʕok (< *-dʕuku) ‘(fire) go out’ > UA *tuka / *tuku / *tuki 'fire go out, dark, black, night’
(900) nʕm ‘be lovely, good, beautiful’ (1289) šgʕ, Hebrew məšuggaʕ ‘raging, mad’ (94) ršʕ ‘act wickedly, be guilty’
> UA *numa / *noma ‘good, well, pretty’ > Nahuatl šiikoaa ‘be jealous, angry’
> UA *tasawa ‘be/do bad’
Many phonemes (sounds) remain much the same, such as t, k, p, s, m, n, etcetera:
(52) Hebrew mukkɛ ‘smitten’ > UA *mukki ‘die, be sick, smitten’
(769) *taqipa (sg), *taqipuu (pl) ‘overpower’ > UA *takipu ‘push’
(750) tmh ‘in awe, fear, speechless’, Syriac tǝmah > UA tuma’ / tu’mï / tehmat / tïhmï ‘be silent, afraid’
(755) Hebrew kutónet ‘shirt-like tunic’
(754) Hebrew participle pone ‘turn to, look’
(851) Hebrew panaa-w ‘face-his’
(852) pl construct paneey- (< *panii) ‘face, surface of’ > UA *pani 'on, on surface of’
(1339) šippaa ‘make smooth’ > UA *sipa / *sippa ‘scrape, shave’
(56) šεkεm / šikm-, Samaritan šekam ‘shoulder’ > UA *sïka ‘shoulder, arm’, Numic *sikum ‘shoulder’
> UA *kutun ‘shirt’
> UA *puni ‘turn, look, see’ > UA *pana ‘cheek, face’
(563) sapat ‘lip’ > UA *sapal ‘lip’
(879) šwy / šawaa ‘broil, roast’ > UA *sawa ‘boil, apply heat, melt'
(1138) Hebrew šor ‘navel’; Arabic surr ‘navel cord’ > Sr ṣuur ‘navel’
(13) snw ‘shine, be beautiful’ > Hopi soniwa ‘be beautiful, bright, brilliant, handsome’ (890) kann ‘shelter, house, nest’ > UA *kanni (NUA) ‘house’ > *kali (SUA) ‘house’ (903) khh, kehah ‘be inexpressive, disheartened’ > UA -kïhahï- ‘sad’
(1045) Hebrew *moškat ‘bracelet, fetter, belt’ (1105) kali / kulyaa ‘kidney’
(1409) Aramaic kuuky-aa’ ‘spider-the’
> Tb mohkat-t ‘belt’
> UA *kali ‘kidney’
> UA *kuukyaŋw ‘spider’; Hopi kòokyaŋw ‘spider’
Semitic emphatic or pharyngealized ṣ > s in UA:
(892) ṣanawbar ‘type of pine tree’
(901) ṣb’ / ṣby / ṣəbee ‘wish, want, seek, delight in’ > UA *supiC ‘like, want’
> UA *sanawaC > Sh sanawap-pin ‘pine tree’ > UA *mos ‘suck’
> UA *sïta / *sïti ‘red’
Semitic emphatic or pharyngealized ṭ > c (ts):
(770) ṭwy / ṭawaa ‘spin (thread)’
(771) ṭʕm ‘taste, eat’ (plural participle ṭoʕmiim) > UA *cu’mi 'suck, sip, kiss'
(772) ṭame’ ‘(be) unclean’, ṭum’a(t) ‘uncleanness, filthy mass’ > UA *co’ma ‘mucus, have a cold’
(832) *sarṭoon ‘scratcher, crab’ > UA *saCtun >*sicu/*suttu ‘claw, fingernail, crab, scratch’
Sometimes the c (ts) lenites (weakens) one more step to s:
(778) ṭibbuur ‘navel’ > NP sibudu; Cr sipu; Hp sipna / sivon- ‘navel’
The p-Northwest Semitic distinguishes x from ђ, as in pre-exilic Hebrew, thus Semitic *x > UA k: (1088) *xld ‘burrow’, xuld / *xild-aa’ ‘mole-the’ > UA *kita ‘groundhog’
(630) *xole ‘be sick, hurting’ > > UA *koli ‘to hurt, be sick’
(631) xmr ‘to ferment’; *xamar ‘wine’; Arabic ximiir ‘drunkard’ > UA *kamaC 'drunk'
(632) *xnk ‘put around the neck’ > UA konaka ‘necklace, string of beads’ (634) *xaṣr- > xaṣṣ ‘hip, haunch, loins’ > UA kaca ‘hip’
Clusters like -m’-, -’m-, -qm-, that is, m clustered with either ’ or q became ŋ in Northern UA, n in SUA: (1246) Old Canaanite sim’al ‘left’, *ha-sim’al ‘the-left’ > Tb aašiŋan ‘left side’ (l > n in NUA)
(1012) šeqma(t) / šiqma(t) ‘sycamore tree’ > UA *sïŋŋa(C) 'cottonwood or aspen tree’
(1144) ’lm ‘be grieved’ > Hebrew ’almaanaa ‘widow’ > UA *o’mana / *oŋani ‘sad, suffering’
Clusters with -r- as 2nd consonant show -Cr- > -Cy-, especially -gr-, -qr- > -ky-, or -gra / -qra > Hopi -kya:
(1173) mwṣ ‘suck’
(1350) ṣd’ / ṣdi ‘grow rusty’
> Nahuatl cawa ‘spin’
(1402) Aramaic moogeraa ‘stored provision’
(1130) Aramaic pagr-aa ‘corpse-the’
(1403) Syriac šigr-aa ‘drain, ditch, gutter-the’
(1405) šqr ‘fair, yellow to red’, Arabic šuqra ‘fair complexion, blondness, redness’ > Hopi sikyà ‘yellow’ (743) *tamar; Aramaic tuumr-aa ‘palm tree-the’ > UA *tu’ya ‘palm tree, sp’
> Hopi mokyàa- ‘bundle, sack of’
> Hopi pïïkya ‘skin, fur’
> Hopi sikya ‘small valley, ravine, canyon with sloped sides’
Proto-Semitic *z > c (ts) in UA:
(1116) Hebrew zέpɛt (< *zipt-) / zaapet ‘pitch’ > UA *copï ‘pitch, resin’
(87) Arabic ʕgz / ʕagaza ‘to age, grow old (of women)’ > Tr wegaca- 'grow old (of women)’
Egyptian terms in UA exceed 400 and have the same sound correspondences as the above Semitic. Egyptian did not include written vowels, only the consonants. Sometimes the vowels are hinted at in transcriptions from other ancient Near-East languages, or from Egyptian’s descendants like Demotic and Coptic, but generally only the consonants are certain. Sometimes the Coptic term is listed along with the Egyptian term, but do not regard Coptic as involved in the Egyptian to UA tie, because UA preserves the Egyptian phonology better than Coptic does usually, though two more millennia removed. Coptic is simply listed for hints at vowels or to show Uto-Aztecan’s better preservation (7.3, p. 331):
Egyptian
(115) sbk / *subak ‘crocodile’
(116) -i ‘old perfective/stative verb suffix’ (117) -w / -iw ‘passive verb suffix’
(124) tks ‘pierce’
(125) km ‘black’
Uto-Aztecan
> UA *supak / *sipak ‘crocodile’ (b > p)
> UA -i ‘intransitive / past / passive/ stative verb suffix’
> UA -wa / -iwa ‘passive verb suffix’
> UA *tïkso ‘pierce, poke’
> UA *koma ‘dark, gray, brown, black’
> UA *nïmi ‘walk around’
> UA *wancio / woncia ‘fox’ (-ns- > -nc- as in sense/cents) > UA *sima ‘go, leave’
(126) nmi ‘travel, traverse’
(129) wnš, pl wnšiw ‘jackal’
(131) šm ‘go, walk, set out, leave’
(219) iqr ‘skillful, excellent, capable, intelligent’ > UA *yikar ‘knowing, intelligent, able, good’ (221) wr ‘great (in size/importance), wrw ‘greatest’ > UA *wïru ‘big’
(222) wnx ‘be clothed, roll of cloth’
(136) win ‘thrust aside, push away, set aside’ (253) spd ‘sharp, be sharp pointed’
(255) sqd ‘slope (of pyramid)’
(210) twt ‘sandal(s)’
(339) t’-ђimat ‘the-wife’; Coptic hime
Note again Egyptian b > UA p, as in the Semitic (132) sbq ‘calf of leg’
(133) sbty ‘enclosure’
(134) qbb ‘cool; calm, quiet, cool breeze’
(137) bbyt ‘region of throat’
(138) bši ‘spit, vomit’, bšw ‘vomit, vomiting’ (139) bnty ‘breast’
(141) bit ‘bee’
(142) bik ‘falcon’
(154) sb’ ‘star’
> UA *wanaC ‘cloth, clothing’
> UA *wina 'throw down/out, spill, empty' > UA *sipaC 'point'
> UA *sikiC ‘slanted (terrain), side’ (q > k) > UA *tuti ‘sandal(s)’
> UA *tïhima 'spouse'
Also Egyptian x > UA *k, as in the Semitic above:
(170) txi ‘be drunk, drink deep’, txw ‘drunkard’ (294) xpš ‘foreleg, thigh’
(295) xpd ‘buttock’
(295) xpdw ‘buttocks’
-
> UA *tïku 'drunk’
-
> UA *kapsi ‘thigh’
-
> UA *kupta ‘buttocks’
-
> UA *kupitu ‘buttocks’
-
> UA *sikun ‘kidney’
-
> UA *sakat / *sakaC ‘grass, willow’
(171) sxn / zxn ‘kidney fat, pancreas’
(174) sxt ‘field, country, pasture, willow’
(178) x’yt / h’yt ‘disease, slaughter, corpse-heap’ > UA *ko’ya ‘die, pl subj; kill, pl obj’
above:
-
> UA *sipika ‘lower leg’
-
> UA *sapti ‘fence of branches’
-
> UA *koppa ‘quiet, calm’
-
> UA *papi ‘larynx, throat, voice’ (b > p)
-
> UA *piso-(ta) ‘vomit’
-
> UA *pitti / *piCti ‘breast’
-
> UA *pitV > *picV 'bee, wasp'
-
> UA *pik ‘hawk species’
-
> UA *sipo’ > *si’po ‘star’
(b > p) (b > p) (b > p) (b > p)
(b > p)
(b > p) (b > p) (b > p)
(247) xr ‘fall’
(320) xpx ‘rob’
(224) wxd ‘be painful, sick, suffer, endure’ (452) xt ‘fire, heat’
> UA *kuri ‘fall’, UA *kara ‘fall’
> UA *kïpïk ‘take, grasp’
> UA *okotï ‘be in pain, suffer, sorrow’ > UA *kut ‘fire’
Egyptian initial pharyngeal ђ > UA *hu, and non-initially ђ > w/o/u:
(180) ђbi ‘be / make festival’ > UA *hupiya ‘sing, song’
(181) ђnqt ‘beer, drinkers’ > UA *hunaka ‘drunk, alcohol’
(182) ђtp / hotpe ‘be gracious, peaceable, set (sun), bury’ > UA *huppi ‘peaceable, go down, sink, dive’
(187) ђw’ ‘foul, putrid, stink, vi’ (188) nђbt ‘nape of the neck, to yoke’ (189) nђb ‘to harness, yoke’
(397) ђti ‘smoke, vapor’
(415) ђnn ‘penis’
> UA *hu’a / *hu’i ‘break wind, stink’ > UA *nohopi > nopi ‘hand, arm’
> UA *noopi ‘carry on back’
> UA *uti 'dew, vapor, frost’
> UA *huna ‘penis’
Egyptian glottal stop ’ > w, or glottal stop next to round vowels, ’ probably causing vowels to round (o, u):
(147) m’i ‘lion’; Coptic mui (148) t’yt ‘shroud’
(198) d’rt ‘bitter gourd’ (205) t’y ‘male, man’
(322) q’i ‘tall, high’; q’yt ‘high land, hill’ (515) ’xi ‘sweep together’
(150) t’ ‘earth, land’; Coptic to
(151) i’w ‘old man’; i’wi ‘be aged’
(153) s’ ‘son’
(259) st’ ‘jar, jug’
(258) st’ ‘drag, pull, pull out, draw’ (154) sb’ ‘star’
(157) it’ ‘take, carry, steal’
(370) ђ’ ‘behind, around’
(431) b’k / b’kt ‘document’
> UA *mawiya ‘mountain lion’
> UA *tawayi ‘cape-like garment’
> UA *sawara 'gourd'
> UA *tawi > *tïwi ‘man, male’
> UA *kawi ‘mountain, rock’
> UA *wak / *wok ‘sweep, comb, brush’ > UA *tïwa / *to’o ‘sand, dust’
> UA *yo’o ‘old’
> UA *so’o ‘child, son’
> UA *soto’i ‘jar’
> UA *(piC)-sutu’a ‘(behind)-pull, drag’ > UA *sipo’ > *si’po ‘star’
> UA *itu’i > i’tu ‘steal, take’
> UA *huwï ‘around’
> UA *po’ok ‘mark, write, tattoo’ (b > p)
Egyptian d corresponds to Semitic ṣ, and thus Egyptian d > UA *s, like Semitic ṣ > UA *s also:
(200) dbt / *dubat ‘brick, adobe brick’ (199) db’ ‘to clothe, garment, clothing’ (198) d’rt ‘bitter gourd’
(197) dʕb ‘coal-black’, dʕbt ‘charcoal’ (194) d’i ‘pierce, transfix’
> UA *supa ‘adobe’
> UA *sipu’ > *si’pu ‘slip, skirt, shirt, clothing’ > UA *sawara 'gourd'
> UA *so’opa ‘black, dark’
> UA *so’a/*so’i 'pierce, sew, shoot arrow'
> UA *suti ‘mosquito, gnat’
(390) dwt ‘mosquito, gnat’
Egyptian initial r- > UA t-, though Tarahumara retains r-:
(164) rn ‘young one, of animals’ (165) rwi ‘dance, v’
(169) rmt ‘man, person’
(167) rwd ‘cord, bow-string’ (337) r’-ib ‘stomach’
Egyptian pharyngeal ʕ > UA *w/o/u: (163) rʕ / rʕw ‘sun’
(162) šʕy ‘sand’; Coptic šoo
(262) ʕnt ‘nail, claw’
(400) sʕr ‘thorn bush(es)’
(426) ʕnr(t) ‘flint’
(464) ʕq ‘enter’
(475) sw ‘it, pronoun’ (is) p’ʕt ‘quail’
> UA *tana ‘offspring’
> UA *tawiya / *tuwiya > *tuya ‘dance’
> UA *tïmati ‘young man’: Tr ŕemarí, Eu temáci- > UA *tïsa ‘rope’
> NUA *to’i ‘stomach’ / SUA *toCpa ‘stomach’
> UA *tawa / *tawi ‘sun, day’ > UA *siwa(l) ‘sand’
> UA *wati 'claw, fingernail'
> UA *sawaro ‘saguaro cactus' > UA *wi’naC ‘flint’
> UA *waka/u ‘enter’ > UA *supa’awi ‘quail’
Like the devoicing of Egyptian b > UA *p, so also is the devoicing of Egyptian d > UA *t, and g > *k:
(268) dwn ‘stretch, straighten; Coptic town (269) dqr ‘fruit’ (> Coptic tiče / jiji)
(270) dbђ ‘ask for’ (Coptic toobh)
(271) dm ‘be sharp, sharpen’; Coptic toom (272) dmi (dmr) ‘touch’
> UA *tuna ‘straight’
> UA *taka(C) ‘fruit’
> UA *tïpiwa / *tïpiN 'ask'
> UA *tama / *tomo ‘be sharp, sharpen’ > UA *tam ‘touch’
(273) dw’ ‘rise early’; dw’w / dw’yt ‘morning’; Coptic to’we > UA *to’i ‘rise, come up/out’ (395) ngg ‘gander/male goose’ > *nakï 'goose' (devoicing of g > k)
Egyptian cluster *-m’- > UA *mw > ŋ in 3 items widespread throughout UA, as in Semitic -m’- > -ŋ- (1246, p. 5):
(280) ђm’ / ђm’t ‘salt’ (> Coptic hmu) (281) sm’ ‘lung’; pl: sm’w ‘lungs’ (284) qm’ ‘create, beget (of father)’
> UA *omwa > *oŋwa / *oŋa ‘salt’
> UA *somwo > *soŋo ‘lungs’
> UA *kumwa > *kuŋa ‘husband’ (q > k)
Other clusters and parallels:
(332) qrђt ‘serpent, partner’ (*qarђat >)
(384) inqt ‘net’
(391) ishb ‘jackal, fox’
(398) k’p ‘cover, close (eyebrows/eyelids)
(434) g’p ‘cut’
(381) wrt ђq’w ‘buzzard’
(404) ђ’dt ‘basket’
(426) ʕnr(t) ‘flint’
(263) šwt ‘shade, shadow’
(264) šmrt ‘large bow’, pl šmrwt
(267) twr ‘reed’
(266) šnw / šni ‘hair, grass’; šni ‘encircle, cover’ > UA *soni / *sono ‘grass, blanket’
(331) qny ‘be yellow’; qnit ‘yellow(ness)’ (333) qd ‘go round, turn, spin’ (> Coptic koote) (446) qm’ ‘fight’; qm’tyw ‘enemies’
(409) nk ‘copulate’
(468) ’wt ‘length’
(470) t’-imnti ‘the west’
(519) wpi ‘open, separate, divide’
> Cp kenekene’e- ‘yellow’ (q > k) > UA *koti / *kuri ‘turn, go around’ (q > k) > UA *kïma’a / *kïmma(n)ci ‘different, enemy’ (q > k) > UA *naka ‘copulate, cover’
> UA *otï / *utu / *uta ‘long, tall’
> UA *tïmïnïmïn ‘north, west’ (reduplicated) > UA *wopa ‘divide’
> UA *koŋwa ‘snake, twin’ (q > k) > UA *ikkaC / *iCkaC ‘carrying net’ (q > k) > UA *isap / *isa’apa ‘coyote’
> UA *kuppa / *kuCpa 'close (eyes)’
> UA *kappi 'break, cut' (devoicing g > k) > UA *wirhukuN 'buzzard, turkey vulture'
> UA *huCta ‘basket’
> UA *wi’naC ‘flint’
> Nahuatl seewal-li ‘shade’
> -samaaloo-t of Nahuatl koo-samaaloo-tl ‘rainbow’ > Nahuatl tool-in ‘cattails, reeds’;
The above 105 Egyptian-UA matches are but 25% of the 400+ listed in the Egyptian section of the book.
The above Semitic and Egyptian parallels in UA both have the same sound correspondences, apparently spoken or
used by one group of people. However, in contrast to those two, a separate sizable set of data suggest another contributing Semitic dialect or language, having a different set of sound correspondences in which Semitic b > UA *kw (like Greek p corresponds to Latin kw), though the Tepiman branch of UA, and Eudeve, Opata, and some dialects of Nahuatl actually have b from Semitic b, though that b corresponds to PUA *kw; so all those UA b = Semitic b in dageshed positions. The data of the kw-Semitic language are what I noticed first, and because the Hebrew b > *p group were exceptions to those correspondences that I noticed first (Hebrew b > PUA *kw), I ignored them for years, but kept them in the back of my mind (not a safe place), until I noticed Egyptian similarities (in UA) whose sound correspondences with UA aligned with those exceptions: that is, Egyptian b > UA *p also, as well as another 40 examples of Semitic b > UA *p, which differences are discussed throughout the book. Not until then did it occur to me that we have two separate Semitic entities that merged in UA—a Phoenician-like kw-Northwest Semitic (kw-NWSem) wherein Semitic b > UA *kw, and an Aramaic-like p-Northwest Semitic (p-NWSem) in which Semitic b > UA p. Furthermore, the p-NWSem speakers seemed to know some Egyptian as well; that is, the p-NWSem and the Egyptian in UA have the same sound correspondences. The data show the two Semitic infusions or languages (kw-NWSem and p-NWSem) to have separate sets of correspondences for other phonemes (basic sounds) as well, the p-NWSem being consistently parallel to the Egyptian correspondences.
Below are some data and sound correspondences from the Phoenician-like kw-Semitic wherein Semitic b > UA *kw:
(4) Hebrew baašel ‘boiled, cook, ripen’ (5) Hebrew bááśaar ‘flesh, penis’
(6) Hebrew baalaʕ ‘swallow’
(7) Semitic *bahamat ‘back’
> UA *kwasïC ‘cook, ripen’
> UA *kwasi ‘tail, penis, flesh’
> UA *kwïluC ‘swallow’
> UA *kwahami ‘back’
> UA *kwïkï ‘cry’ (from kw-NWSemitic) > UA *kwiya / *kwira 'earth'
> UA *kwiyam ‘be lazy, do lackadaisically’ > UA *cikwa ‘rain
> UA *tïkwi ‘say, talk, speak’
(r > y/i)
(r > y/i) (r > y/i)
(r > y/i)
(24) bky / bakaay ‘cry’
(19) barr- ‘land (as opposed to sea)’
(27) brm ‘worn out, weary, bored with’
(1457) Arabic ṣabba ‘pour, drip, overflow’
(11) Hebrew -dabber ‘speak’
(26) Hebrew bεn ‘son’; pl: bəneey ‘children (of)’ > Nahuatl *konee 'child, offspring’
As in all three languages, the voiced pharyngeal ʕ > w/o/u:
(88) ʕlq ‘stick, adhere’, ʕalaqat ‘leech’
(89) śeeʕaar ‘hair’; Arabic šaʕr / šaʕar ‘hair’ (92) yáʕar ‘wood, forest, thicket’
> UA *walaka ‘snail’ (of similar slimy adhering texture) > UA *suwi ‘body hair’ (r > y/i) > UA *yuwi / yuyi ‘evergreen species’ (r > y/i)
Kw-Semitic non-initial -r- > -y-/-i- (also in 5, 19, 27, 89, 92 above) and tends to raise & front the preceding vowel (V > i):
(62) srq / saraq ‘to comb’
(65) mrr ‘pass, go, walk’
(64) Semitic krr / krkr ‘go in circles, dance’ (19) barr- ‘land (as opposed to sea)’
(27) brm / baram ‘worn out, weary, bored with’ (79) Hebrew ђmr ‘cover with, smear on’
(81) Hebrew ђabéret ‘wife’
> UA *siyuk / *ciyuk ‘to comb’ > UA *miya ‘go’
> SP kiya ‘have a round dance’ > UA *kwiya / *kwira 'earth'
> UA *kwiyam ‘be lazy, do lackadaisically’ > UA *humay ‘smear, spread, rub, paint’
> UA *hupi 'woman, wife'
(-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-) (-r- > -y-/-i-)
As in the Egyptian and the p-NWSemitic contributions, so also in the kw-NWSemitic, ђ > hu or w/o/u:
Kw-Semitic ṣ > UA c (ts):
(83) Hebrew ṣrђ ‘cry, roar’ > UA *cayaw ‘yell’ (84) Hebrew ṣmђ, imperfective: yi-ṣmaђ ‘sprout’ > UA *icmo ‘sprout’
> UA *huc ‘arrow’
> UA *humay ‘smear, spread, rub, paint’ > UA *uppa ‘bathe, wash, rub’
> UA *hupi 'woman, wife'
> UA *husi / *hwasi ‘look, peek at’
> NUA *wïkkwiN- ‘wrap around, coil’
(78) Hebrew ђeṣ ‘arrow’
(79) Hebrew ђmr ‘cover with, smear on’
(80) Hebrew ђbb ‘rub off, wash’
(81) Hebrew ђabéret ‘wife’
(82) Hebrew ђzy / ђazaa ‘see, behold, look’
(658) ђbl ‘bind’, *-ђabbil ‘bind’
(853) Aramaic ђippušit-aa ‘beetle-the’; Arabic *xunpus > UA *wippusi ‘beetle’ (-np- > -pp- in both Aramaic & UA) In the next section are three more examples (83, 84, 85) of ђ > w/o.
(85) Hebrew ṣlђ ‘rush, v’
(899) ṣinw-, pl aṣnaa’ ‘twin, one twin’
(29) ṣəbii > ṣəvii ‘gazelle’
(86) ṣʕq ‘shout, call out, cry (out)’, ṣǝʕaaqaa ‘yell, call, n’ > UA *coaka ‘cry’ (28) ṣurṣur ‘cricket’ > UA *corcor ‘cricket’
(78) ђeṣ ‘arrow’ > UA *huc ‘arrow’
Unlike its associated rounding in p-Semitic, the kw-Semitic glottal stop ’ is not rounded and often lost:
(991) Hebrew ni-qra’ ‘he/it is called/named’ (587) ’argaamaan ‘purple, red-purple’ (1214) Hebrew mee-’ayn ‘from where?’ (1055) ’aamaqqǝt-aa ‘lizard-the, n.f.’
> UA *nihya ‘call, name’
> UA *aNkaC ‘red’
> Tb maa’ayn ‘where from’
> UA *makkaCta(Nka) ‘horned toad’ > UA *tïma ‘earth’
(591) ’adaamaa / ’adaamaa ‘earth’
(592) Hebrew ’abneṭ, pl: ’abneṭ-iim ‘sash, girdle’ > UA *natti ‘belt’
(1054) raqbubit ‘moth, decayed, moth-eaten’ > UA *...kupïpika / *(C)Vkupïpika 'butterfly'
> UA *coloa ‘flee, run’ > UA *cono’o 'twin(s)' > Hopi cöövi- ‘antelope’
Kw-Semitic final or non-initial -l also tends to raise and front vowels (V > e, i):
(1225) Hebrew ’abaal ‘truly, indeed’ > Tr abe ‘yes, an emphatic’
(54) Hebrew taapel ‘whitewash’; Aramaic ṭǝpel ‘plaster’ > UA *tïpi ‘white clay’
(1321) Hebrew ђargol, Arabic *ђargal / *ђurgul ‘locust’ > Tr urugi-pari ‘type of grasshopper’
(798) Hebrew ’akal ‘(he/it) ate’ (perfective) > UA *’aki ‘open mouth, eat, take/put into one’s mouth’ (797) Hebrew *yo’kal ‘(he/it) eats’ (imperfective) > UA *yï’ïki 'swallow, taste, finish'
Number 797 (-l raising -a- > -i-) is in contrast to p-NWSemitic *tukkaC wherein final -l has no raising effect. (796) Hebrew *to’kal ‘(she/it) eats’ > UA *tukkaC > Num *tïkkaC 'eat'
Such a tripartite combination I first considered suspect until the quantity for each grew to more than sufficient to allow each to stand on its own strength, as each dimension has 400-700 sets, and two of the three have the same sound correspondences. Should we ignore the strength of a case of 1500 similarities? Or should we be fair and consider the data when a few hundred items support each dimension of the tripartite scenario? If one simply cannot bear the thought of the three, then pick only one of the groups: Egyptian with 400+ sets; p-NWSem with about 700 sets; or kw-NWSem with about 400 sets. Ought a correlation of 400 sets be ignored? Even 400 sets is three times what most Native American language families were founded on.
(r > y/i) (r > y/i)
Yet a few words of caution are in order: (1) First of all, linguists would look dimly on a tripartite collection of languages to propose an Old World tie with an American language family. Linguistically, each of those three has to stand on its own merit, independent of the other two. Yet the numbers of similarities for each are enough data for each one of the three to do exactly that—serve as a valid case each in and of itself (400 to 700 similarities for each).
(2) Anthropologists and linguists are wary and weary of hearing about proposed ties between Semitic or Egyptian and New World languages—about 300 years’ worth of weary. Most such claims have been bogus to borderline or amateurish at best, somewhat justifying linguists’ wariness in light of claims void of sound methodology, that is, lacking what linguists have found to be established principles and patterns for verifying language relatedness: rules of sound change that create consistent sound correspondences, hundreds of vocabulary matches consistent with those sound correspondences, and some grammatical and morphological alignments, which sum constitutes the comparative method. Thus, the language similarities in the book are presented within a framework of sound correspondences. In fact, the Semitic or Egyptian forms proposed to underlie the UA forms often answer questions and explain puzzles in UA that Uto- Aztecanists have not yet been able to explain; and explanatory power is a cherished quest among linguists. While the finds do seem significant, some details remain to be worked out.
(3) Given the amount of Egyptian vocabulary in UA, we might expect to find and may yet identify more Egyptian grammatical patterns in UA. However, if the Egyptian phrasing in UA is reduced as much as many Egyptian phrases are reduced in Coptic (a late form of Egyptian dating to 2,000 years ago), then such identifications would be a challenge (if even possible), requiring time, not to mention requiring a greater depth of familiarity with UA languages and Egyptian than yet exists in any single mind. Many living languages reduce as drastically. In American English, one often hears ‘hwəjədu?’ for ‘what did you do?’ Therein -j- is the phonological reduction of the final -t of ‘what’, the whole of ‘did’, and the y- of ‘you’—some of three words (-t did y-) reduced to one consonant (-j-).
Often as drastic were Egyptian changes to Coptic: Egyptian iwr-ti > Coptic εετ (eet) ‘pregnant’ (Loprieno 1995, 78); the i/y is not obvious, nor w or r. Practically nothing of the stem ‘pregnant’ (iwr) is left, only a long vowel and the t of the stative suffix. Egyptian r-di.t iri.f sdm > Coptic e-t-ref sotem ‘to cause that he may do hearing’—a reduction of 8 consonants (r-di.t iri.f) to (etref) 3 consonants and 2 vowels (Cerny and Groll 1993, 155), though 3 of the original 8 consonants are vowel-like or semi-vowels. Egyptian tw.i m nʕy r sdm ‘I am in going to hear’ (= I shall hear) became Coptic tinasotm, or tw.i m nʕy r > tina (Cerny 1976, 104), 8 segments (sounds) to 4. Adding to the challenge is that the time depth from Late Egyptian to Coptic is half the probable time depth in this problem: if UA is partially from Egyptian, the Egyptian in the UA languages is now being recorded at a time depth a millennium or two greater than the time depth between Late Egyptian and Coptic. Yet UA preserves many vowels and details better than Coptic (7.3 in book).
On the other hand, these data explain many things previously unexplained in UA:
(1) The phonology of medial (middle) consonant clusters is a huge problem in UA, yet Semitic and Egyptian shed light on many of those clusters and explain the mutual effect of adjacent consonants on each other. For example, (614) makteš ‘mortar, grinding stone’ > UA *ma’ta ‘mortar, grinding stone’ in most of UA, but the noun made verb Ca mataš (< *mattaš) ‘crush, squash, vt’ shows final -š and a medial cluster which became geminated *-tt-, as *-t- > -l- in Cahuilla.
(2) Uto-Aztecanists agree on each UA language’s reflex that corresponds to PUA *p. (A language’s reflex is its corresponding sound which the proto-sound changed to.) However, five UA languages—Tarahumara, Mayo, Yaqui, Arizona Yaqui, and Eudeve—show both initial b and p corresponding to PUA *p. This split is usually ignored as an inconvenient inconsistency in these languages. However, the initial b forms in these languages correspond to Egyptian b or Semitic b of p-NWSemitic, and the initial p forms to Semitic/Egyptian p. How can such an alignment be coincidental? For the various UA forms of b vs. p to match Semitic/Egyptian b vs. p is significant (see 6.2 in the book).
(3) PUA initial *t (at the beginning of words) corresponds to the initial t of most UA languages, except for Tarahumara initial r. So if PUA *t became Tarahumara r, then where does Tarahumara initial t come from? The data in this work suggest that Semitic/Egyptian initial r became t, so in most UA languages initial r and initial t merged to look like PUA *t, but Tarahumara kept them separate. Thus, 6.1 (in the book) clarifies the Tarahumara r vs. t puzzle.
(4) Other matters in UA at 6.3, 6.4, and 6.5 (in the book) are also explained by the Near-East language ties. Many UA features match reconstructable Hebrew/Phoenician better than they match other Semitic languages:
Uto-Aztecan Hebrew Arabic
(1) *-ima (pl suffix) Semitic masc pl: *-iima -uuna/-iina
Aramaic Akkadian -iin -uu
-aat -aat
--
yǝθeb
(904) *-te (pl suffix)
-
(2) *na-
-
(3) *yasipa
Semitic fem pl: *-ooteey -aat reciprocal/passive: *na- in-
‘sit / dwell’ *yašiba waθaba 10
Significant is the language parallel of Yiddish, the language of the Jewish peoples of Central Europe. Uto- Aztecan and Yiddish are both Semitic infusions into non-Semitic areas, where each (as a minority people) borrowed heavily from the languages of the larger surrounding peoples. Originally coming out of Palestine, many Jewish people sojourned in Greece, Rome, and elsewhere along the northern Mediterranean, then some among them expanded into central Europe, where their original Hebrew-and-Aramaic idiom borrowed mostly from German, but also from Slavic and other languages of their successive environments through which they traveled and periodically settled (Kriwaczek 2006, 40-48; Harshaw 1990, 5-7). Thus, Yiddish is a transplant and very much a language mix (like English and many languages are). Estimates generally have 15-20% of Yiddish being from the original Hebrew-Aramaic vocabulary, and 80-85% borrowed from German, etc. Similarly, only 15% of Old English continued into modern English; the other 85% was lost, being replaced by words from French, Latin, and other languages from which we English speakers borrowed (Baugh and Cable 55). While the details of Uto-Aztecan’s prehistory may yet require lifetimes to unlock, Uto-Aztecan seems to have a higher percentage of its basic vocabulary from Near-Eastern languages than Yiddish has. For example, Yiddish pronouns are all from German, whereas most UA pronouns match Semitic (see book’s section 3 on pronouns). Most Yiddish body-part terms are from German—kop (head), oig (eye), oi’er (ear), hant (hand), hartz (heart), k’nee (knee), fus (foot), etcetera—while a higher percentage of UA body-part terms, animal terms, and basic nouns of nature match Semitic or Egyptian (see section 7.1 in the book).
The two forms of Semitic are both Northwest Semitic, though quite distinguishable to a degree, but not entirely. Two separate sets of sound correspondences distinguish most of the vocabulary as noted previously, but not all. Some details remain to be clarified. While the kw-Semitic exhibits Phoenician-Hebrew like features and the p-Semitic has Aramaic-like features and vocabulary, it also has Hebrew-like features. These kinds of unique sets of features are fairly typical of related languages. For example, the language of the Book of Job is unique: though labeled Hebrew, it contains features more Arabic-like and Aramaic-like than the Hebrew of the other authors. The language of the Nabateans, though primarily an Aramaic dialect, was also more Arabic-like than other Aramaic dialects. So any diffused offshoot can be expected to be a unique combination of features.
Regarding the Aramaic leaning of the p-Semitic, some scholars (Young 1993, 54-62, 85-86) note that Aramaic did influence the dialects of ancient Israel, especially northern Israel. What is not known is the degree or extent, though it may have been more significant or pervasive than presently known. These data may be relevant to that void in present knowledge. Marsha White (1997), in a review of Young 1993, summarizes Young’s substance more clearly and concisely than either I or Young could: “Young ... suggests that Biblical Hebrew goes back to the adaptation of the pre- Israelite Canaanite prestige language.... Thus, from the beginning of Israelite history there were two linguistic strata: literary/formal and dialectical/colloquial. This situation of diglossia persisted throughout pre-exilic Israelite history.... The best explanation for ... so many Aramaisms in the early literary language is that they were in the lower (i.e., spoken) form of the language, and that Archaic Biblical Hebrew was open to elements from the underlying dialects. The strong presence of Aramaisms in the oldest Biblical Hebrew undermines the theory that Aramaisms equals late” (White 1997).
The Aramaic-like p-NWSemitic aligns well with the likelihood of Aramaic substrata serving as underlying dialects to the literary language of Canaanite/Hebrew, perhaps throughout the Northern Kingdom’s centuries. What language did the mothers of the Israelites (Leah and Rachel) speak? Aramaic! In addition, Aramaic was somewhat a lingua franca throughout most of the area through most centuries. So did the Israelites really set aside Aramaic upon entering Canaan? Or did they adopt degrees of bilingualism while adding the Phoenician/Canaanite literary language? The latter is likely nearer the case in some areas, if not most.
The UA basic vocabulary from Egyptian and Semitic are numerous: body parts, plant and animal terms, nouns of nature (sun, moon, star, sky, rock, water, etc.; see 7.1 in the book.) A considerable amount of Semitic morphology or fossilized items of Semitic verb conjugations are found in UA. Below are three groups; many more are at 7.6 in the book.
(1420) Semitic nwr ‘to make/become light’ with infinitive and imperfective: -nuur(u), and perfective naar > UA has both in Eu nurú ‘to dawn, become light’ and Tbr nare ‘to dawn, become light’
Uto-Aztecan has four separate forms from the verb bky /bakaa ‘to cry, weep’:
(559) p-Semitic bky/ bakaa ‘he cried, wept’; Syriac bakaa / baka’ > UA *paka’ ‘cry’
(24) kw-Semitic bky/ bakaa ‘he cried, wept’; Hebrew baakaa > UA *kwïkï / *o’kï 'cry’
Because bilabials as first segment in a cluster disappear (-bk- > -k-) in Egyptian/Semitic > UA, the imperfective 3rd person masculine singular *ya-bkV ‘he/it weeps’ with imperfective prefix originally *ya- (later yi-) also matches UA *yakka (560) Semitic *ya-bkay ‘he/it weeps, cries, masc sg.’ > UA *yaCkaC > *yakka / *yaka ‘cry’
(561) Semitic *ta-bkay ‘she/it weeps, cries, fem sg.’ > UA *takka > NP taka ‘cry’.
(1094) impfv -ktoš (< *-ktusu) ‘pound, grind’
(615) *kitteš (< *kittaš) ‘grind’
(614) makteš ‘mortar, grinding stone’
Of interest is the noun made verb Ca mataš ‘crush, squash, vt’ showing final -š and a medial cluster or geminated *-tt-, because single *-t- would become -l- in Cahuilla. At 7.6 in the book are many more groups of cognate sets reflecting different forms of Semitic’s elaborate conjugation systems (see 1.21).
(332) Egyptian qrђt ‘serpent’, Egyptian qrђ ‘friend, partner’ > UA/Nahuatl koŋwa ‘snake, twin’
(406) Egyptian b’ ‘ram, soul’ > UA *pa’a ‘mountain sheep, all living beings’
(98) Hebrew rqʕ ‘stamp, beat out (metal), spread out’; Hebrew raaqiiaʕ ‘extended surface, expanse, sky’
(994) Ls qáya/i- ‘blow down (a tree)’ (which is the same result as ‘uproot’)
and Ls qáya/i- ‘heal’ are listed as separate verbs in the Luiseño dictionary, though phonologically identical, yet the corresponding Syriac verb ʕqr also means both ‘uproot’ and ‘heal’ (ʕǝqar or -ʕqar > qayV).
(734) Hebrew mə-ṣuudat ‘net, prey’ i.e., game > UA *masat / *masot ‘deer’;
(720) Hebrew nebεl ‘skin-bottle, skin’ in the common phrase of Hebrew nebεl yayin ‘skin of wine’;
Syriac nbl / n’bl > Classical Nahuatl no’pal-li ‘prickly pear’ which was used to make alcoholic beverage; so as Semitic ‘skin/bottle’ (container) came to mean the fermentable substance in UA, so also ‘the bottle’ is used for alcoholic reference in English too!
*tusu ‘grind’ with loss of 1st C in a cluster Yq kitte / kittasu ‘grind’
*ma’ta ‘mortar, grinding stone’
(724) Semitic parʕoš ‘flea (jumper)’ (from the Semitic verb prʕš ‘jump’) > UA *par’osi / *paro’osi ‘jackrabbit’; the jackrabbit, like the flea, is also a jumper, and in UA *paro’osi ‘jackrabbit’ we see all 4 consonants and 2 identical vowels in two of the most extraordinary jumpers of the animal kingdom.
References (The full 18-page, 600-source bibliography is in Exploring the Explanatory Power of Egyptian and Semitic in Uto-Aztecan) Allen, James P. 2010. Middle Egyptian: An Introduction to the Language and Culture of the Hieroglyphs. 2nd ed.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Arends, Jacques, Silvia Kouwenberg, and Norval Smith. 1995. Theories focusing on the non-European input. In Pidgins and Creoles:
An Introduction. Eds. Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken, and Norval Smith, 99-110. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Bakker, Peter, and Pieter Muysken. 1995. Mixed languages and language intertwining. In Pidgins and Creoles: An Introduction.
Eds. Jacques Arends, Pieter Muysken, and Norval Smith, 41-52. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Baugh, Albert C., and Thomas Cable. 1978. A History of the English Language, 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.
Blau, Joshua. 1998. Topics in Hebrew and Semitic Linguistics. Jerusalem: Magnes Press.
Campbell, Lyle. 1997. American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America. New York: Oxford University Press. Cerny, Jaroslav. 1976. Coptic Etymological Dictionary. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chamberlain, Alexander Francis. 1888. The Catawba Language. Toronto: Imrie and Graham.
Comprehensive Aramaic Lexicon. Cincinnati: Hebrew Union College and Jewish Institute of Religion. Online database.
Faulkner, Raymond O. 1962. A Concise Dictionary of Middle Egyptian. London: Griffith Institute at the Oxford University Press.
Haas, Mary. 1958. Algonkian-Ritwan: the end of a controversy. IJAL 24:159-73.
Hale, Kenneth. 1962. Jemez and Kiowa correspondences in reference to Kiowa-Tanoan. IJAL 28:1-5.
Hale, Kenneth. 1967. Toward a reconstruction of Kiowa-Tanoan phonology. IJAL 33:112-120.
Hannig, Rainer. 1995. Die Sprache der Pharaonen: Grosses Handwörter Buch: Ägyptisch – Deutsch. Band 64. Mainz: Philipp von Zabern. Harshaw, Benjamin. 1990. The Meaning of Yiddish. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Hill, Kenneth C. 2012. Review of Uto-Aztecan: A Comparative Vocabulary, by Brian Stubbs. IJAL 78/4 (2012), 591-3.
Kerler, Dov-Ber. 1999. The Origins of Modern Literary Yiddish. Oxford Modern Languages and Literature Monographs. Oxford University Press. Klar, Kathryn Ann. 1977. Topics in historical Chumash grammar. Ph.D dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Koehler, Ludwig, and Walter Baumgartner. 1994. The Hebrew and Aramaic Lexicon of the Old Testament. Revised by Walter Baumgartner and
Johann Jakob Stamm; translated and edited under the supervision of M.E.J. Richardson. Leiden: E. J. Brill. Kriwaczek, Paul. 2006. Yiddish Civilization: The Rise and Fall of a Forgotten Nation. New York: First Vintage Books Edition. Lane, Edward W. 1863-1893. An Arabic-English Lexicon. Online. London: Williams and Norgate.
Rendsburg, Gary A. 1997. Chapter 5 “Ancient Hebrew Phonology.” Online offprint from
Phonologies of Asia and Africa. Alan S. Kaye, ed. Eisenbrauns. Read online December 12, 2013.
Sapir, Edward. 1913, 1915. Southern Paiute and Nahuatl: a study in Uto-Aztecan, parts 1 and 2. Part 1, 1913 in Journal de la Société des
Américanistes de Paris 10:379-425. Part 2, 1915 in American Anthropologist 17:98-120, 306-328, reprinted 1919 in JSAP 11: 443-88. Siebert, Frank. 1945. Linguistic classification of Catawba. IJAL 11:100-4,211-8.
Smith, R. Payne, and J. Payne Smith. 1999. A Compendious Syriac Dictionary. From the Thesaurus Syriacus of R. Payne Smith.
Edited by J. Payne Smith (Mrs Margoliouth). Ancient Language Resources. Published by Oxford University Press, 1902.
Republished in Eugene, Oregon: Wipf and Stock Publishers, 1999.
Sokoloff, Michael. 2002. Dictionary of Jewish Babylonian Aramaic of the Talmudic and Geonic Periods.
Baltimore and London: John Hopkins University Press and Ramat-Gan, Israel: Bar Ilan University Press. Stubbs, Brian D. 2015. Exploring the Explanatory Power of Egyptian and Semitic in Uto-Aztecan.
Stubbs, Brian D. 2011. Uto-Aztecan: A Comparative Vocabulary. Rocky Mountain Books and Productions, Blanding, UT;
Shumway Family History Services. Flower Mound, Texas.
Taylor, Allan R. 1963. “Comparative Caddoan.” IJAL 29:113-31.
Voegelin, C.F., F.M. Voegelin, and Kenneth L. Hale. 1962. Typological and Comparative Grammar of Uto-Aztecan.
Indiana University Publications in Anthropology and Linguistics: Memoir 17, supplement to IJAL 28(1).
Wehr, Hans. 1976. A Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic. Ed. J. Milton Cowan. New York: Spoken Language Services, Inc.
Weinreich, Max. 1980. History of the Yiddish Language. Translated by Shlomo Noble, with Joshua A Fishman. Chicago: The University of Chicago
Press. First published in Yiddish as Geshikhte fun der Yidisher shprakh, By YIVO Institute for Jewish Research, 1973.Wexler, Paul. 1993. The Ashkenazic Jews: A Slavo-Turkic People in Search of a Jewish Identity. Slavica Publishers, Inc.
White, Marsha. 1997. Review of Diversity in Pre-Exilic Hebrew, by Ian Young. In Journal of Biblical Literature, vol. 116, no. 4.
Young, Ian. 1993. Diversity in Pre-Exilic Hebrew. Tübingen: J.C.B. Mohr-Siebeck.
Background: Brian’s interest in languages followed a two-year attempt to learn Navajo, which made everything else seem easier. He was first a Semiticist, taking Hebrew, Arabic, and Egyptian courses during his B.A. from BYU. He began graduate work in Semitic languages (Hebrew, Arabic, Aramaic) at the University of Utah. A professor suggested that he take a linguistics course, which he found so interesting that he switched to linguistics, and completed an M.A. in linguistics. U of U was a primary center for Uto-Aztecan (UA) studies at the time, providing Brian a good foundation in comparative UA. During that time he could not help but notice a few hundred similarities between UA and Semitic, with sound correspondences, etc. After an M.A. in linguistics, he resumed his studies in Near Eastern languages and completed the coursework and comprehensive exams for a PhD (ABD) in Semitic languages and linguistics, though his primary research interests remained in UA. After publishing a few articles in the International Journal of American Linguistics and elsewhere, he decided that articles are too haphazard a way of scattering one’s ideas to the four winds with hopes that subsequent scholars would have the patience to gather them together for a cohesive picture of one’s thoughts on a matter—too optimistic and not likely. So he finished a three-decade effort to produce the comparative reference book Uto-Aztecan: A Comparative Vocabulary. Brian’s UA works preceding this book have been well received by other UA specialists. While the arrival of this Near-East tie with UA has most wishing to ignore it, a brave few have voiced very positive assessments.
Roger William Wescott, first in his Princeton class, PhD in linguistics, Rhodes Scholar at Oxford, President of the Linguistic Association of Canada and the United States, author of 500 articles and 40 books, called Brian’s work “a strong link between the Uto- Aztecan and Afro-Asiatic languages”. David H. Kelley, Harvard PhD who published in anthropology, linguistics, Uto-Aztecan, and contributed to the decipherment of the Mayan glyphs, said upon receiving an earlier draft: “The thick thing came in the mail and I did not want to tackle it, but dutifully opened it, intending to look at a page or two. However, I started to read and ended up reading the whole thing. It is the most interesting and significant piece of research I have seen in years.” Mary Ritchie Key, and two PhD linguists specializing in UA, all spoke well of it. John S. Robertson, a leading Mayanist and Harvard trained PhD in historical linguistics, also speaks highly of the strength of this case.